1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281
|
"""
======================================================================
Repeated measures ANOVA on source data with spatio-temporal clustering
======================================================================
This example illustrates how to make use of the clustering functions
for arbitrary, self-defined contrasts beyond standard t-tests. In this
case we will tests if the differences in evoked responses between
stimulation modality (visual VS auditory) depend on the stimulus
location (left vs right) for a group of subjects (simulated here
using one subject's data). For this purpose we will compute an
interaction effect using a repeated measures ANOVA. The multiple
comparisons problem is addressed with a cluster-level permutation test
across space and time.
"""
# Authors: Alexandre Gramfort <alexandre.gramfort@telecom-paristech.fr>
# Eric Larson <larson.eric.d@gmail.com>
# Denis Engemannn <denis.engemann@gmail.com>
#
# License: BSD (3-clause)
import os.path as op
import numpy as np
from numpy.random import randn
import matplotlib.pyplot as plt
import mne
from mne.stats import (spatio_temporal_cluster_test, f_threshold_mway_rm,
f_mway_rm, summarize_clusters_stc)
from mne.minimum_norm import apply_inverse, read_inverse_operator
from mne.datasets import sample
print(__doc__)
###############################################################################
# Set parameters
# --------------
data_path = sample.data_path()
raw_fname = data_path + '/MEG/sample/sample_audvis_filt-0-40_raw.fif'
event_fname = data_path + '/MEG/sample/sample_audvis_filt-0-40_raw-eve.fif'
subjects_dir = data_path + '/subjects'
src_fname = subjects_dir + '/fsaverage/bem/fsaverage-ico-5-src.fif'
tmin = -0.2
tmax = 0.3 # Use a lower tmax to reduce multiple comparisons
# Setup for reading the raw data
raw = mne.io.read_raw_fif(raw_fname)
events = mne.read_events(event_fname)
###############################################################################
# Read epochs for all channels, removing a bad one
# ------------------------------------------------
raw.info['bads'] += ['MEG 2443']
picks = mne.pick_types(raw.info, meg=True, eog=True, exclude='bads')
# we'll load all four conditions that make up the 'two ways' of our ANOVA
event_id = dict(l_aud=1, r_aud=2, l_vis=3, r_vis=4)
reject = dict(grad=1000e-13, mag=4000e-15, eog=150e-6)
epochs = mne.Epochs(raw, events, event_id, tmin, tmax, picks=picks,
baseline=(None, 0), reject=reject, preload=True)
# Equalize trial counts to eliminate bias (which would otherwise be
# introduced by the abs() performed below)
epochs.equalize_event_counts(event_id)
###############################################################################
# Transform to source space
# -------------------------
fname_inv = data_path + '/MEG/sample/sample_audvis-meg-oct-6-meg-inv.fif'
snr = 3.0
lambda2 = 1.0 / snr ** 2
method = "dSPM" # use dSPM method (could also be MNE, sLORETA, or eLORETA)
inverse_operator = read_inverse_operator(fname_inv)
# we'll only use one hemisphere to speed up this example
# instead of a second vertex array we'll pass an empty array
sample_vertices = [inverse_operator['src'][0]['vertno'], np.array([], int)]
# Let's average and compute inverse, then resample to speed things up
conditions = []
for cond in ['l_aud', 'r_aud', 'l_vis', 'r_vis']: # order is important
evoked = epochs[cond].average()
evoked.resample(50, npad='auto')
condition = apply_inverse(evoked, inverse_operator, lambda2, method)
# Let's only deal with t > 0, cropping to reduce multiple comparisons
condition.crop(0, None)
conditions.append(condition)
tmin = conditions[0].tmin
tstep = conditions[0].tstep
###############################################################################
# Transform to common cortical space
# ----------------------------------
#
# Normally you would read in estimates across several subjects and morph them
# to the same cortical space (e.g. fsaverage). For example purposes, we will
# simulate this by just having each "subject" have the same response (just
# noisy in source space) here.
#
# We'll only consider the left hemisphere in this tutorial.
n_vertices_sample, n_times = conditions[0].lh_data.shape
n_subjects = 7
print('Simulating data for %d subjects.' % n_subjects)
# Let's make sure our results replicate, so set the seed.
np.random.seed(0)
X = randn(n_vertices_sample, n_times, n_subjects, 4) * 10
for ii, condition in enumerate(conditions):
X[:, :, :, ii] += condition.lh_data[:, :, np.newaxis]
###############################################################################
# It's a good idea to spatially smooth the data, and for visualization
# purposes, let's morph these to fsaverage, which is a grade 5 ICO source space
# with vertices 0:10242 for each hemisphere. Usually you'd have to morph
# each subject's data separately, but here since all estimates are on
# 'sample' we can use one morph matrix for all the heavy lifting.
# Read the source space we are morphing to (just left hemisphere)
src = mne.read_source_spaces(src_fname)
fsave_vertices = [src[0]['vertno'], []]
morph_mat = mne.compute_source_morph(
src=inverse_operator['src'], subject_to='fsaverage',
spacing=fsave_vertices, subjects_dir=subjects_dir, smooth=20).morph_mat
morph_mat = morph_mat[:, :n_vertices_sample] # just left hemi from src
n_vertices_fsave = morph_mat.shape[0]
# We have to change the shape for the dot() to work properly
X = X.reshape(n_vertices_sample, n_times * n_subjects * 4)
print('Morphing data.')
X = morph_mat.dot(X) # morph_mat is a sparse matrix
X = X.reshape(n_vertices_fsave, n_times, n_subjects, 4)
###############################################################################
# Now we need to prepare the group matrix for the ANOVA statistic. To make the
# clustering function work correctly with the ANOVA function X needs to be a
# list of multi-dimensional arrays (one per condition) of shape: samples
# (subjects) x time x space.
#
# First we permute dimensions, then split the array into a list of conditions
# and discard the empty dimension resulting from the split using numpy squeeze.
X = np.transpose(X, [2, 1, 0, 3]) #
X = [np.squeeze(x) for x in np.split(X, 4, axis=-1)]
###############################################################################
# Prepare function for arbitrary contrast
# ---------------------------------------
# As our ANOVA function is a multi-purpose tool we need to apply a few
# modifications to integrate it with the clustering function. This
# includes reshaping data, setting default arguments and processing
# the return values. For this reason we'll write a tiny dummy function.
#
# We will tell the ANOVA how to interpret the data matrix in terms of
# factors. This is done via the factor levels argument which is a list
# of the number factor levels for each factor.
factor_levels = [2, 2]
###############################################################################
# Finally we will pick the interaction effect by passing 'A:B'.
# (this notation is borrowed from the R formula language). Without this also
# the main effects will be returned.
effects = 'A:B'
# Tell the ANOVA not to compute p-values which we don't need for clustering
return_pvals = False
# a few more convenient bindings
n_times = X[0].shape[1]
n_conditions = 4
###############################################################################
# A ``stat_fun`` must deal with a variable number of input arguments.
#
# Inside the clustering function each condition will be passed as flattened
# array, necessitated by the clustering procedure. The ANOVA however expects an
# input array of dimensions: subjects X conditions X observations (optional).
#
# The following function catches the list input and swaps the first and the
# second dimension, and finally calls ANOVA.
#
# .. note:: For further details on this ANOVA function consider the
# corresponding
# :ref:`time-frequency tutorial <sphx_glr_auto_tutorials_plot_stats_cluster_time_frequency_repeated_measures_anova.py>`. # noqa: E501
def stat_fun(*args):
# get f-values only.
return f_mway_rm(np.swapaxes(args, 1, 0), factor_levels=factor_levels,
effects=effects, return_pvals=return_pvals)[0]
###############################################################################
# Compute clustering statistic
# ----------------------------
#
# To use an algorithm optimized for spatio-temporal clustering, we
# just pass the spatial connectivity matrix (instead of spatio-temporal).
# as we only have one hemisphere we need only need half the connectivity
print('Computing connectivity.')
connectivity = mne.spatial_src_connectivity(src[:1])
# Now let's actually do the clustering. Please relax, on a small
# notebook and one single thread only this will take a couple of minutes ...
pthresh = 0.0005
f_thresh = f_threshold_mway_rm(n_subjects, factor_levels, effects, pthresh)
# To speed things up a bit we will ...
n_permutations = 128 # ... run fewer permutations (reduces sensitivity)
print('Clustering.')
T_obs, clusters, cluster_p_values, H0 = clu = \
spatio_temporal_cluster_test(X, connectivity=connectivity, n_jobs=1,
threshold=f_thresh, stat_fun=stat_fun,
n_permutations=n_permutations,
buffer_size=None)
# Now select the clusters that are sig. at p < 0.05 (note that this value
# is multiple-comparisons corrected).
good_cluster_inds = np.where(cluster_p_values < 0.05)[0]
###############################################################################
# Visualize the clusters
# ----------------------
print('Visualizing clusters.')
# Now let's build a convenient representation of each cluster, where each
# cluster becomes a "time point" in the SourceEstimate
stc_all_cluster_vis = summarize_clusters_stc(clu, tstep=tstep,
vertices=fsave_vertices,
subject='fsaverage')
# Let's actually plot the first "time point" in the SourceEstimate, which
# shows all the clusters, weighted by duration
subjects_dir = op.join(data_path, 'subjects')
# The brighter the color, the stronger the interaction between
# stimulus modality and stimulus location
brain = stc_all_cluster_vis.plot(subjects_dir=subjects_dir, views='lat',
time_label='Duration significant (ms)',
clim=dict(kind='value', lims=[0, 1, 40]))
brain.save_image('cluster-lh.png')
brain.show_view('medial')
###############################################################################
# Finally, let's investigate interaction effect by reconstructing the time
# courses
inds_t, inds_v = [(clusters[cluster_ind]) for ii, cluster_ind in
enumerate(good_cluster_inds)][0] # first cluster
times = np.arange(X[0].shape[1]) * tstep * 1e3
plt.figure()
colors = ['y', 'b', 'g', 'purple']
event_ids = ['l_aud', 'r_aud', 'l_vis', 'r_vis']
for ii, (condition, color, eve_id) in enumerate(zip(X, colors, event_ids)):
# extract time course at cluster vertices
condition = condition[:, :, inds_v]
# normally we would normalize values across subjects but
# here we use data from the same subject so we're good to just
# create average time series across subjects and vertices.
mean_tc = condition.mean(axis=2).mean(axis=0)
std_tc = condition.std(axis=2).std(axis=0)
plt.plot(times, mean_tc.T, color=color, label=eve_id)
plt.fill_between(times, mean_tc + std_tc, mean_tc - std_tc, color='gray',
alpha=0.5, label='')
ymin, ymax = mean_tc.min() - 5, mean_tc.max() + 5
plt.xlabel('Time (ms)')
plt.ylabel('Activation (F-values)')
plt.xlim(times[[0, -1]])
plt.ylim(ymin, ymax)
plt.fill_betweenx((ymin, ymax), times[inds_t[0]],
times[inds_t[-1]], color='orange', alpha=0.3)
plt.legend()
plt.title('Interaction between stimulus-modality and location.')
plt.show()
|