1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429
|
---
title: "Automated Interpretation of Indices of Effect Size"
output:
rmarkdown::html_vignette:
toc: true
fig_width: 10.08
fig_height: 6
tags: [r, effect size, rules of thumb, guidelines, interpretation]
vignette: >
\usepackage[utf8]{inputenc}
%\VignetteIndexEntry{Automated Interpretation of Indices of Effect Size}
%\VignetteEngine{knitr::rmarkdown}
editor_options:
chunk_output_type: console
bibliography: bibliography.bib
---
```{r message=FALSE, warning=FALSE, include=FALSE}
library(knitr)
options(knitr.kable.NA = "")
knitr::opts_chunk$set(comment = ">")
options(digits = 2)
```
## Why?
The metrics used in statistics (indices of fit, model performance, or parameter
estimates) can be very abstract. A long experience is required to intuitively
***feel*** the meaning of their values. In order to facilitate the understanding
of the results they are facing, many scientists use (often implicitly) some set
of **rules of thumb**. Some of these rules of thumb have been standardize and
validated and subsequently published as guidelines. Understandably then, such
rules of thumb are just suggestions and there is nothing universal about them.
The interpretation of **any** effect size measures is always going to be
relative to the discipline, the specific data, and the aims of the analyst. This
is important because what might be considered a small effect in psychology might
be large for some other field like public health.
One of the most famous interpretation grids was proposed by **Cohen (1988)** for
a series of widely used indices, such as the correlation **r** (*r* = .20,
small; *r* = .40, moderate and *r* = .60, large) or the **standardized difference** (*Cohen's d*). However, there is now a clear evidence that Cohen's
guidelines (which he himself later disavowed; Funder, 2019) are much too
stringent and not particularly meaningful taken out of context
[@funder2019evaluating]. This led to the emergence of a literature discussing
and creating new sets of rules of thumb.
Although **everybody** agrees on the fact that effect size interpretation in a
study should be justified with a rationale (and depend on the context, the
field, the literature, the hypothesis, etc.), these pre-baked rules can
nevertheless be useful to give a rough idea or frame of reference to understand
scientific results.
The package **`effectsize`** catalogs such sets of rules of thumb for a
variety of indices in a flexible and explicit fashion, helping you understand
and report your results in a scientific yet meaningful way. Again, readers
should keep in mind that these thresholds, as ubiquitous as they may be,
**remain arbitrary**. Thus, their use should be discussed on a case-by-case
basis depending on the field, hypotheses, prior results, and so on, to avoid
their crystallization, as for the infamous $p < .05$ criterion of hypothesis
testing.
Moreover, some authors suggest the counter-intuitive idea that *very large effects*, especially in the context of psychological research, is likely to be a
"gross overestimate that will rarely be found in a large sample or in a
replication" [@funder2019evaluating]. They suggest that smaller effect size are
worth taking seriously (as they can be potentially consequential), as well as
more believable.
## Correlation *r*
There can be used to interpret not only Pearson's correlation coefficient, but also Spearman's, $\phi$ (phi), Cramer's *V* and Tschuprow's *T*. Although Cohen's *w* and Pearson's *C* are _not_ a correlation coefficients, they are often also interpreted as such.
#### @funder2019evaluating
```r
interpret_r(x, rules = "funder2019")
```
- **r < 0.05** - Tiny
- **0.05 <= r < 0.1** - Very small
- **0.1 <= r < 0.2** - Small
- **0.2 <= r < 0.3** - Medium
- **0.3 <= r < 0.4** - Large
- **r >= 0.4** - Very large
#### @gignac2016effect
Gignac's rules of thumb are actually one of few interpretation grid justified
and based on actual data, in this case on the distribution of effect magnitudes
in the literature.
```r
interpret_r(x, rules = "gignac2016")
```
- **r < 0.1** - Very small
- **0.1 <= r < 0.2** - Small
- **0.2 <= r < 0.3** - Moderate
- **r >= 0.3** - Large
#### @cohen1988statistical
```r
interpret_r(x, rules = "cohen1988")
```
- **r < 0.1** - Very small
- **0.1 <= r < 0.3** - Small
- **0.3 <= r < 0.5** - Moderate
- **r >= 0.5** - Large
#### @evans1996straightforward
```r
interpret_r(x, rules = "evans1996")
```
- **r < 0.2** - Very weak
- **0.2 <= r < 0.4** - Weak
- **0.4 <= r < 0.6** - Moderate
- **0.6 <= r < 0.8** - Strong
- **r >= 0.8** - Very strong
#### @lovakov2021empirically
```r
interpret_r(x, rules = "lovakov2021")
```
- **r < 0.12** - Very small
- **0.12 <= r < 0.24** - Small
- **0.24 <= r < 0.41** - Moderate
- **r >= 0.41** - Large
## Standardized Difference *d* (Cohen's *d*)
The standardized difference can be obtained through the standardization of
linear model's parameters or data, in which they can be used as indices of
effect size.
#### @cohen1988statistical
```r
interpret_cohens_d(x, rules = "cohen1988")
```
- **d < 0.2** - Very small
- **0.2 <= d < 0.5** - Small
- **0.5 <= d < 0.8** - Medium
- **d >= 0.8** - Large
#### @sawilowsky2009new
```r
interpret_cohens_d(x, rules = "sawilowsky2009")
```
- **d < 0.1** - Tiny
- **0.1 <= d < 0.2** - Very small
- **0.2 <= d < 0.5** - Small
- **0.5 <= d < 0.8** - Medium
- **0.8 <= d < 1.2** - Large
- **1.2 <= d < 2** - Very large
- **d >= 2** - Huge
#### @gignac2016effect
Gignac's rules of thumb are actually one of few interpretation grid justified
and based on actual data, in this case on the distribution of effect magnitudes
in the literature. These is in fact the same grid used for *r*, based on the
conversion of *r* to *d*:
```r
interpret_cohens_d(x, rules = "gignac2016")
```
- **d < 0.2** - Very small
- **0.2 <= d < 0.41** - Small
- **0.41 <= d < 0.63** - Moderate
- **d >= 0.63** - Large
#### @lovakov2021empirically
```r
interpret_cohens_d(x, rules = "lovakov2021")
```
- **r < 0.15** - Very small
- **0.15 <= r < 0.36** - Small
- **0.36 <= r < 0.65** - Moderate
- **r >= 0.65** - Large
## Odds Ratio (OR)
Odds ratio, and *log* odds ratio, are often found in epidemiological studies.
However, they are also the parameters of ***logistic*** regressions, where they
can be used as indices of effect size. Note that the (log) odds ratio from
logistic regression coefficients are *unstandardized*, as they depend on the
scale of the predictor. In order to apply the following guidelines, make sure
you *standardize* your predictors!
Keep in mind that these apply to Odds *ratios*, so Odds ratio of 10 is as
extreme as a Odds ratio of 0.1 (1/10).
#### @chen2010big
```r
interpret_oddsratio(x, rules = "chen2010")
```
- **OR < 1.68** - Very small
- **1.68 <= OR < 3.47** - Small
- **3.47 <= OR < 6.71** - Medium
- **OR >= 6.71 ** - Large
#### @cohen1988statistical
```r
interpret_oddsratio(x, rules = "cohen1988")
```
- **OR < 1.44** - Very small
- **1.44 <= OR < 2.48** - Small
- **2.48 <= OR < 4.27** - Medium
- **OR >= 4.27 ** - Large
This converts (log) odds ratio to standardized difference *d* using the
following formula [@cohen1988statistical;@sanchez2003effect]:
$$
d = log(OR) \times \frac{\sqrt{3}}{\pi}
$$
## Coefficient of determination (R<sup>2</sup>)
### For Linear Regression
#### @cohen1988statistical
```r
interpret_r2(x, rules = "cohen1988")
```
- **R2 < 0.02** - Very weak
- **0.02 <= R2 < 0.13** - Weak
- **0.13 <= R2 < 0.26** - Moderate
- **R2 >= 0.26** - Substantial
#### @falk1992primer
```r
interpret_r2(x, rules = "falk1992")
```
- **R2 < 0.1** - Negligible
- **R2 >= 0.1** - Adequate
### For PLS / SEM R-Squared of *latent* variables
#### @chin1998partial
```r
interpret_r2(x, rules = "chin1998")
```
- **R2 < 0.19** - Very weak
- **0.19 <= R2 < 0.33** - Weak
- **0.33 <= R2 < 0.67** - Moderate
- **R2 >= 0.67** - Substantial
#### @hair2011pls
```r
interpret_r2(x, rules = "hair2011")
```
- **R2 < 0.25** - Very weak
- **0.25 <= R2 < 0.50** - Weak
- **0.50 <= R2 < 0.75** - Moderate
- **R2 >= 0.75** - Substantial
## Omega / Eta / Epsilon Squared
The Omega squared is a measure of effect size used in ANOVAs. It is an estimate
of how much variance in the response variables are accounted for by the
explanatory variables. Omega squared is widely viewed as a lesser biased
alternative to eta-squared, especially when sample sizes are small.
#### @field2013discovering
```r
interpret_omega_squared(x, rules = "field2013")
```
- **ES < 0.01** - Very small
- **0.01 <= ES < 0.06** - Small
- **0.06 <= ES < 0.14** - Medium
- **ES >= 0.14 ** - Large
#### @cohen1992power
These are applicable to one-way ANOVAs, or to *partial* Eta / Omega / Epsilon
Squared in a multi-way ANOVA.
```r
interpret_omega_squared(x, rules = "cohen1992")
```
- **ES < 0.02** - Very small
- **0.02 <= ES < 0.13** - Small
- **0.13 <= ES < 0.26** - Medium
- **ES >= 0.26** - Large
## Kendall's coefficient of concordance
The interpretation of Kendall's coefficient of concordance (*w*) is a measure of
effect size used in non-parametric ANOVAs (the Friedman rank sum test). It is an
estimate of agreement among multiple raters.
#### @landis1977measurement
```r
interpret_omega_squared(w, rules = "landis1977")
```
- **0.00 <= w < 0.20** - Slight agreement
- **0.20 <= w < 0.40** - Fair agreement
- **0.40 <= w < 0.60** - Moderate agreement
- **0.60 <= w < 0.80** - Substantial agreement
- **w >= 0.80** - Almost perfect agreement
## Cohen's *g*
Cohen's *g* is a measure of effect size used for McNemar's test of agreement in
selection - when repeating a multiple chose selection, is the percent of matches
(first response is equal to the second response) different than 50%?
#### @cohen1988statistical
```r
interpret_cohens_g(x, rules = "cohen1988")
```
- **d < 0.05** - Very small
- **0.05 <= d < 0.15** - Small
- **0.15 <= d < 0.25** - Medium
- **d >= 0.25** - Large
## Interpretation of other Indices
`effectsize` also offers functions for interpreting other statistical indices:
- `interpret_gfi()`, `interpret_agfi()`, `interpret_nfi()`, `interpret_nnfi()`,
`interpret_cfi()`, `interpret_rmsea()`, `interpret_srmr()`, `interpret_rfi()`,
`interpret_ifi()`, and `interpret_pnfi()` for interpretation CFA / SEM
goodness of fit.
- `interpret_p()` for interpretation of *p*-values.
- `interpret_direction()` for interpretation of direction.
- `interpret_bf()` for interpretation of Bayes factors.
- `interpret_rope()` for interpretation of Bayesian ROPE tests.
- `interpret_ess()` and `interpret_rhat()` for interpretation of Bayesian
diagnostic indices.
# References
|